What is the process through which people get to understand the social norms

Beniamino Cislaghi, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Department of Global Health and Development, 15-17 Tavistock Place, WC1H 9SH, London, United Kingdom E-mail: [email protected]

Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).

Abstract

The theoretical literature on social norms is multifaceted and at times contradictory. Looking at existing reviews, we aimed to offer a more complete understanding of its current status. By investigating the conceptual frameworks and organizing elements used to compare social-norms theories, we identified four theoretical spaces of inquiry that were common across the reviews: what social norms are, what relationship exists between social norms and behavior, how social norms evolve, and what categories of actors must be considered in the study of social norms. We highlight areas of consensus and debate in the reviews around these four themes and discuss points of agreement and disagreement that uncover trajectories for future empirical and theoretical investigation.

Keywords: social norms, reviews, reference group, cross-disciplinary

Few concepts in the social sciences are as fundamental and cross-disciplinary as the concept of social norms, commonly understood as the unwritten rules shared by members of the same group or society (). The study of social norms spans several disciplines, including philosophy (), sociology (), social and moral psychology (; ; ; ), law (), economics (), anthropology (), gender studies (), health sciences (; ; ), communication studies (), environmental studies (), political science (), finance (), marketing (), and information technology (). The concept of social norms already populated the work of early philosophers, such as Aristotle (), as well as later ones, such as Thomasius (), Locke (), ), and many thereafter. However, the allusions to norms existed in religious doctrines and philosophical knowledge that preceded Aristotle by thousands of years ().

The cross-disciplinary manifestation of the social norms concept has meant the literature on what norms are and how they affect people’s actions has grown in very different directions and today includes several, often conflicting, theories. A few scholars have taken on the task of putting order to it, the result being a series of reviews. Most reviews, however, tend to look at the social-norms literature in two disciplines at most, leaving those who intend to engage in cross-disciplinary conversations without a common language and understanding. Many begin with a caveat similar to the one from : “Given space limitations, it is impossible to provide a comprehensive account of . . . [the social norms] literature” (p. 360).

There also exists a considerable body of theoretical and empirical work that is not formally part of the social-norms literature but that has strong conceptual linkages to it. For instance, theoretical and empirical studies in anthropology, sociology, and gender studies have looked extensively at theoretical concepts such as, to cite three examples, socialization (the process through which individuals learn the norms of a given society; ), acculturation (the process through which an individual adapts another culture’s norms; ), or structural ritualization (the dynamics through which collective practices acquire symbolic significance; ). Similar concepts are connected to norms, even though they are not explicitly included in the work of those authors who specifically studied social norms dynamics. Because they are a critical component of the grammar of society (), social norms are closely interwoven with other important processes (as the three we mentioned) and social and psychological concepts, such as attitudes (one’s individual preference about something; ), factual beliefs (one’s beliefs about how the physical world functions; ), or self-efficacy and group efficacy (one’s beliefs about one’s or one’s group’s capacity to achieve a given goal; ). These various constructs contribute to creating a web of meanings that affects how people feel, think, and act. Together, they occupy a large space of investigation in that multidisciplinary system of theories usually referred to as social theory (; ).

Although establishing definitive common ground across social-norms theories might be impossible given the disciplinary distance between some of these theories, the opportunity exists to increase awareness of current debates across disciplines and theories by comparing and contrasting existing reviews, laying the ground for further research on social norms to engage with broader social theory. In this article, we provide a map of the social-norms literature by comparing existing reviews and highlighting areas of agreement and disagreement emerging from these reviews.

Method

Our larger aim was to look at how different reviews had organized the social-norms literature, particularly how reviews of social-norms theory had classified, compared, and analyzed theories from different disciplines. Following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, we searched five databases (PubMed, ProQuest, Web of Science, JSTOR, and Cochrane) for articles that reviewed social-norms theories from one or multiple disciplinary perspectives. We included articles that (a) were written in English; (b) were published in either a peer-reviewed journal or as gray literature; (c) explicitly mentioned social norms (for instance, articles on social influence or gender norms were excluded); and (d) organized the social-norms literature by comparing theories from two or more disciplines (articles that exclusively examine social norms within sociology were excluded). Empirical articles were not included unless they contained a review of different theoretical approaches to social norms (as in the case of ).

We used the following search terms: social norms; (social norms OR social norm) AND (review OR theor* OR model*); social norm* AND overview; norm* AND review; and norm* AND concept*. Our initial scoping of the literature produced 624 records. We added 52 records from the gray literature that were identified through snowballing. After removing duplicates, we had 412 records. We screened these records and shortlisted 57 of them. We assessed the full-text articles for eligibility and rated the records independently, arriving at a list of 30 articles. We then confronted our rankings, resolved disagreements, and decided on a case-by-case basis for the articles that were not explicitly marked as reviews, obtaining the final 22 studies included in this qualitative synthesis. Table 1 provides an overview of these studies as an indication of the discipline from which they originated as well as the aim to which they were written.

Table 1.

Overview of Articles Included in the Analysis

ReferenceDisciplinePurpose/descriptionPhilosophyTo uncover how the “normativity of norms plays an indispensable role in accounting for the motive to comply with them” (p. 172)Social psychology“Provide a brief orientation to behavioral science scholarship about norms” (p. 721)Health sciences“Undertake a review of the literature on social norms to identify many of the large number of proposed social mechanisms by which norms fulfill the function of social control” (p. 28)Bicchieri & Muldoon (2014)PhilosophyReviews “early theories” and “game-theoric accounts” of social normsBusiness managementReviews the social norms literature to “investigate whether various informal constraints—as manifested in social norms and social cohesion—are related to firm-level corporate governance” (p. 42)Economics“Provide an overview of recent work that shows how to incorporate norms into economic models, and how they affect the dynamics of economic adjustment” (p. 313)Communication science“Summarize . . . how different disciplines have approached the study of norms” (p. 1)Social psychologyReviews the relevant literature on social norms, conformity and complianceBusiness management“Review work on social norms, with a particular emphasis on organizationally relevant theories and findings, in order to offer insight into directions for future research” (para. 2)Legal studiesTo examine “the core concepts of law and socio-economics and the importance of these for the understanding of social norms in legal studies” (p. 159)SociologyTo address “three short-comings in the conceptual treatment of norms: (1) a lack of agreement in generic definitions, (2) no adequate classificatory scheme for distinguishing types of norms, and (3) no consistent distinction between attributes of norms that are true by definition and those that are contingent” (p. 586)Communication scienceTo identify “factors for consideration in norms-based research to enhance the predictive ability of theoretical models” (p. 127)International developmentTo offer “an account of what social norms and other social practices are” (p. 4)Information technology“The objectives of this paper are (i) to review and discover the current state of norms architecture and the normative processes, (ii) to propose a norm’s life cycle model based on the current state of norms research, and (iii) to propose potential future work in norms and normative multiagent research” (p. 1)Business management“Review and integrate norm constructs from different literatures into a general framework” (p. 2)Legal studiesReviews theories on “the emergence, stabilization, weakening, and changing of social norms” (p. 3)Health sciencesReviews literature on “social norms theory and its application to health behavior change” (p. 265)Psychology“To review the nature, origins, and theories of prosocial norms” (p. 1)Legal studies“To understand and defend the place of law in norm management” (p. 907)International developmentTo “review the landscape of theory around social norms” (p. 5)Villatoro (2010)Artificial intelligence“To capture the different definitions and points of view of social norms from the related research areas and adapt them to a multiagent perspective” (p. 2)EconomicsTo review “how social norms evolve and how norm shifts take place using evolutionary game theory as the framework of analysis” (p. 360)

Open in a separate window

Four key themes emerged from the data that were relevant to the aim set forth herein. We present these four themes in the Results section and ask the following four questions. First, what are the definitions of social norms included in the reviews (i.e., what are social norms)? Second, what pathways of normative influence are commonly identified in the literature? Third, what types of mechanisms are described in the reviews for how social norms come about, evolve, and dissipate? And fourth, what categories of agents are identified in the reviews as relevant in the study of social norms? In the next section, we look at results for each of these themes in detail before discussing their relevance and offering some concluding remarks on future potential trajectories for research on social norms.

Results

What are social norms?

The social-norms literature is characterized by a great variety of definitions and theoretical approaches with regard to what constructs are considered social norms. Here, we present points of consensus and debate across reviews on what social norms are and what they are not.

Consensus and debate on what social norms are not

Although little universal consensus exists on what social norms are, much more exists on what they are not. Table 2 summarizes the areas of implicit and explicit consensus and debate about what social norms are and are not.

Table 2.

Areas of Consensus and Debate Across Reviews

Consensus


DebateSocial norms are not . . .Social norms are . . .Instinctual or biological reactions (; ; )“Social” and shared by some members of a group (; ; ; Bicchieri & Muldoon, 2014; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; )Social norms are individual constructs (; ; ; ; ; )Personal tastes (; )Related to behaviors and inform decision making (; ; ; Bicchieri & Muldoon, 2014; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; )Social norms are collective constructs (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; )Personal habits (; )Capable of affecting the health and well-being of groups of people (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; )Social norms are a combination of both individual and collective constructs (; ; ; )Behavioral regularities in a group due to demographic trends, common choices made under very limited options, or the aggregation of individuals with similar tastes (; ; ; ; )Prescriptive or proscriptive (; Bicchieri & Muldoon, 2014; ; )

Open in a separate window

Note: Theoretical positions that were either explicitly mentioned by reviewers or implied by their definitions of social norms are summarized in the first column.

Reviewers tend to agree that social norms are not instinctual or reactive behaviors such as crying while cutting onions, shivering from walking out in the cold, or running away from wild dogs barking in a street at night (; ). Social norms are also different from personal tastes (e.g., liking lemon sorbet; ; ). Reviews also specify that social norms are not personal habits, such as putting glasses in their case on the bedside table before going to sleep.

Social norms are also not simple behavioral regularities in groups of people (). Some behavioral regularities can be attributed to norms, whereas others may be the result of nonnormative factors (; ; ; ). Nonnormative factors shaping behavioral regularities include environmental factors (a decrease in workers’ productivity because of heat waves), policy or technological changes (an increase in the number of people working into older age following improvements in the health-care system of a country), and scarcity of choice for other reasons (Irish people eating potatoes during the famine that hit Ireland in the 19th century). Nonnormative regularities can also be attributed to individual characteristics and tastes. People tend to interact with those with whom they share a particular interest—for instance, Japanese cinema enthusiasts will autonomously join a Japanese cinema society. This process, through which people with a common taste join together to pursue their interest, leads to similarities within a group that are not due to norms but to personal preferences, a phenomenon commonly referred to as homophily ().

Consensus and debate on what social norms are

Across the reviews, we found three points of consensus on what social norms are. First, most agree that social norms must be “social” in some sense (although, as discussed below, they disagree on what this means). Second, most reviewers agree that social norms inform action-oriented decision making in some way (as we detail further below). Finally, most reviews mention that social norms can affect people’s health and well-being. A majority note that social norms can be beneficial to cooperation and to social order (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). However, although social norms can help people live together, focusing exclusively on their positive functions limits the potential of social-norms theory to explain the persistence of harmful practices and behaviors (; ; ). A smaller number of reviews explicitly discuss social norms that are harmful. These reviews examine norms that (a) encourage a variety of unhealthy behaviors such as drinking alcohol, smoking cigarettes, or sharing needles (; ; ); (b) result in harmful practices such as child marriage () or female genital mutilation/cutting (); or (c) sustain discriminatory practices such as feuding norms (), norms authorizing violence in genocides (), and caste norms ().

Health-and-development practitioners have been mostly interested in using social-norms theory to investigate why people comply with harmful health-related practices and what can be done to change their actions. Sociologists and moral psychologists have instead offered a large body of work on the benefits and evolutionary advantages of prosocial norms. We find it important to look at both positive and negative effects of complying with norms. Discarding their positive effect might make us mindless of the critical role that social norms play in human societies; health interventions should not aim to “remove” social norms in an attempt to make people more independent of others. That is not only impossible but (as the evidence above suggests) also harmful to people’s well-being. At the same time, norms can be harmful; studying why people comply with these norms and how can they be changed can equip policymakers with important strategies to improve people’s health and well-being.

Despite the points of consensus mentioned above, profound theoretical disagreement exists on what norms are. As we mentioned, reviewers disagree on what it means for norms to be social. To some reviewers, norms are social because they stem from human interactions (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ), whereas other reviewers define them as social because they are expectations about other people’s beliefs and behaviors (; ), because they hold social meaning (; ; ; ), or because they allow the functioning of the social structure (; ). We found further sources of disagreement among the reviews. One major distinction that emerged in our analysis is whether social norms are an individual or collective construct.

As individual constructs, social norms are understood to be psychological states of individuals, such as beliefs or emotions. As collective constructs, they are understood to be conditions or features of social groups or structures. In Table 3, we grouped the definitions of social norms provided across the literature that fall into either conceptual category (individual or collective constructs).

Table 3.

Social Norms as Individual and Collective Constructs

Construct
(social norms as . . .)DefinitionReviewed byIndividual levelBeliefs (perceptions or expectations)What an individual holds true about others in the social group and/or about what others in the social group do or believe; ; ; ; ; ; and Feelings or emotionsPositive or negative emotional reactions to the idea of an action; ; ; and Interpretations of collective rulesAn individual’s understanding of a societal or collective rule/what a collective rule means to an individual and A kind of motivationA reason for actingCollective levelSocial phenomenon of a groupA fact or situation that can be observed in a social group or community; ; and Behavioral regularitiesA pattern of behavior that can be observed at the level of a population; ; ; ; ; ; and Collective or group beliefsBeliefs ascribed to a social group, community, or collective of individuals; ; ; ; and SanctionsSocial reactions punishing norm violations or rewarding conformity to norms; ; ; ; ; and Rules, standards, guidesStatements that assign a value to an action or way of behaving (e.g., obligation, permissibility, appropriateness, prohibition) that are recognized in a society or social group; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; and EquilibriumAn existing state in a population in which no one individual or group is motivated to change the situationBicchieri & Muldoon (2014); ; ; and

Open in a separate window

Although many reviewers recognize that different definitions exist in the literature, some privilege one type of construct in their definition. Specifically, six reviews focus mainly on theories that define social norms as individual constructs (; ; ; ; ; ). Most theories of norms as individual constructs define them as the beliefs of an individual of what is common (what people do in situation X) and approved (the extent to which people approve of those who do Y in situation X) in a given group or society. Seminal here is the work by , who call beliefs of the first type descriptive norms and beliefs of the second type injunctive norms. A few reviewers mention emotions as part of their conception of social norms. Most refer to feelings in passing, but one review () explicitly defines prosocial norms as prosocial feelings.

By contrast, 10 reviews privilege theories of social norms as collective constructs, that is, external (as opposed to internal) forces affecting people’s actions. These constructs include, for instance, shared or institutionalized community rules that are part of the cultural ethos of a group (such as monogamous or polygamous family structures) or behavioral patterns observed within groups and societies (e.g., voter turnout; see Table 3; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). Finally, 6 reviews include both individual and collective constructs of social norms in their analyses (; ; ; ; ; ).

It comes as no surprise that the social-norms literature includes theories that look at norms as either individual or collective constructs or that strive to integrate the two. Both approaches have their own benefits. Understanding social norms as individual constructs is more appropriate to the study of the psychological mechanisms underlying normative phenomena. We found, for instance, greater advantages of using a norm as individual constructs in public-health research and action as well as in targeted behavioral-change interventions in international development. Approaches that look at social norms as individuals’ beliefs were widely used to design effective programmatic and measurement strategies for health promotion (). On the other hand, theories that define social norms as collective constructs will be helpful to researchers investigating how norms operate and diffuse through time at the population level, as it might happen, for instance, in historical and anthropological studies. Integrating the two approaches might be helpful in finally uncovering their dialectically reciprocal influence, as some reviewers have themselves suggested (; ; ). Researchers interested in conducting cross-disciplinary work on social norms, such as studying how people’s normative beliefs are embodied and influenced by formal institutions (such as the education system or the family), will likely benefit from approaching both streams of thought on what social norms are. Research in international development, for instance, has often focused on the mechanisms through which social norms influence community practices and could be complemented by the understanding of how social norms are embedded in national economic and political structures and how they interact with broader processes of change.

What pathways of normative influence are commonly identified in the literature?

Across the reviews we found further disagreement, mostly reflecting disciplinary boundaries, on the relation between norms and behavior. This disagreement specifically related to (a) whether reviewers consider one or multiple pathways of influence from norm to action and (b) whether they understand norms as “direct” or “indirect” (see below) sources of influence.

Reviews that consider one normative pathway include, for example, the suggestion that social norm compliance is exclusively motivated by the presence or anticipation of positive or negative sanctions () or by the simultaneous presence of both empirical and normative expectations (two concepts not too conceptually distant from, respectively, descriptive and injunctive norms; ). One implication of this position is that without the required sanctions or beliefs in the case of Bicchieri et al. (2018), the reviewers assume that compliance will not follow from the social norm. Other reviewers, instead, suggest multiple pathways to compliance. These reviewers recognize that norms can translate into action in a variety of situations and under different conditions. Some look at descriptive and injunctive norms as two pathways of influence: Descriptive norms offer information people can use to orient their actions, whereas injunctive norms put pressure on people to meet other people’s expectations (; ; ; ; ). Others investigate an even wider array of pathways of normative influence. One review (), for example, presented four pathways: uncertainty (e.g., people look at what others do when they are unsure about what is the best course of action), identity (e.g., people comply with social norms to express membership in a group), reward (e.g., people anticipate rewards for compliance), and enforcement (e.g., the group forces individuals into compliance).

The second main difference in how reviews explain how norms affect behavior is whether they understood social norms as direct or indirect sources of influence. Norms are direct sources of influence when they alone are sufficient to direct behavior (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). As an example, think of a person who joins a bus line simply because he or she knows that others do so. Changes in norms that exert direct influence should, logically, result in a change in people’s behavior: If people stop queuing for the bus, latecomers will not queue either. By contrast, when a norm is an indirect source of influence, it intersects with one or multiple intermediary factors to cause that action (; ; ; ; ; ; ). For example, suggest that norms lead to an intermediary element, a behavioral intention, that is mediated by various behavioral, individual, and contextual factors that can either strengthen or reduce the influence of a norm. These factors include, for instance, the perceived cost of acting in accordance with the norm or the time constraints the individual faces in making a choice about how to behave. When a norm exerts indirect influence, changing norms may not be sufficient to change behavior because the ecology of factors sustaining that behavior might still hold.

In addition to the two differences above, we also found that reviewers disagree on the specific pathways that lead from norm to action. Three pathways in particular emerged from our analysis. According to these pathways, norms affect behavior by providing value-neutral information, creating external obligations, and becoming internal obligations.

Norms providing value-neutral information

Some social norms provide neutral information about what action is common, indicating practical or efficient courses of action for what the individual had set out to do (e.g., navigate a new city, use public transportation, feed oneself, coordinate with others). Social norms that provide information are often referred to as either descriptive norms (; ; ; ), empirical expectations (), or collective expectations (). Norms motivate actions by providing information in situations in which

  1. People must choose between different value-neutral courses of action and do not have a strong preference for either alternative (e.g., “Since everyone is walking on the left side of the sidewalk, I will also walk on the left side”; ; ; ).

  2. People use benchmarks or points of reference as heuristic standards of what they should achieve in life and when (e.g., “I aspire to have my first child by 30 because that is when most people normally have their first child in my social group”; ).

  3. People try to figure out the most efficient courses of action to achieve a concrete goal (e.g., “If everyone else drives to work, it must be the most effective way of getting there”; ; ; ; ; ; ).

  4. People need a convention to allow their interaction (e.g., “Everyone speaks English at this meeting, so I will speak English too”; ; ; ; ).

When they provide information, norms do not necessarily affect people’s attitudes, as opposed to when they create external obligations.

Norms creating external obligations

In addition to providing neutral information, social norms can exert pressure on individuals to act in a specific way (; ; ; ). When norms follow this pathway to action, people consider the possible positive or negative consequences that will follow their compliance or lack of it (; ; ; ; ; ; ). These consequences can be economic (e.g., having to pay a fee for violating the norm; ; ; ; ); reputational (e.g., being ostracized by other family members after getting divorced because they consider divorce unacceptable; ; ; ; ; ); and emotional (e.g., feeling shame when arriving underdressed at a party; ; ; ). External influence can take many forms, including

  1. Role modeling (e.g., celebrities in the media marketing compliance with a given norm as a sign of fashionable attractiveness; ; ; ; ).

  2. Social pressure (e.g., adolescent peers pressuring a friend to smoke), subtle encouragement (e.g., parents complimenting their sons for being brave and their daughters for being pretty), and active enforcement (e.g., teachers or religious leaders using violence to punish norm violators) that occur before or after one acts in accordance to or in violation of a norm (; ; ; ).

  3. Anticipation of (as opposed to actual) rewards and penalties, including the anticipation of social approval or disapproval (e.g., anticipation of gossip or the desire to be seen as a good marriage partner) and being accepted in or excluded from a given social group (e.g., the group of the “cool” kids at school or the intellectuals in a village; ; ; ; ; ; ). The threat or anticipation of punishment can sometimes be enough (e.g., fear of violence for violating a norm; ; ).

Norms that create external obligations do not need to be aligned with individuals’ attitudes to motivate compliance. The term pluralistic ignorance refers to cases in which most people disagree with a norm but comply with it because they do not know the extent to which others also disapprove of it. Similar discrepancies between a group’s norm and group members’ individual attitudes have raised the interest of those who looked at new avenues for harm reduction; they suggest that interventions could uncover pluralistic ignorance by correcting people’s misperceptions of what others approve of, eventually reducing compliance with the harmful practice sustained by the norm (; ; ; ; ). However, when these interventions are not well designed they can inadvertently have negative consequences. Take, for instance, interventions that intend to increase awareness of a given harmful practice in the general population. With the purpose of shocking the audience, these interventions might unwittingly publicize the spread of a harmful norm (e.g., 80% of first-year students experience sexual violence in university campuses), ultimately generating a boomerang effect that would increase the very harmful behavior that these interventions are trying to reduce ().

Norms becoming internal obligations

Compliance with social norms can be motivated by internal factors and preferences (; ; ). In this case, people recognize the validity of the norm in itself and comply with it because of the value they attach to it rather than because they anticipate consequences for complying with it or not (). The process through which people assimilate social norms to the point that they become internally driven motivations is often referred to as internalization (; ; ; ; ). When they are internalized, social norms shape an individual’s beliefs about how they should act (). On this pathway, people follow the norm from then on, even when others around them do not, which is why some reviewers call these norms personal whereas others go so far as to call them moral. Several reviewers disagree that these moral and personal norms can be considered social norms, precisely because of their internal character (; ; ; ; ). People comply with a norm on this pathway because (a) they believe it embodies their values (; ; ); (b) compliance contributes to their self-understanding or identity (; ; ; ); or (c) a lack of alternatives impinges people’s capacity to envision change, inducing them to comply willingly with the norm because they view it as the only option available (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ), for example, women who wear high heels at work because they have never seen working women dressed differently (an example included in ).

Some reviewers divide these three pathways (i.e., providing value-neutral information, creating external obligations, and becoming internal obligations) into separate categories. However, we suggest that they are, in practice, intersecting and nonexclusive. Although their separation (as the one we offered) can be helpful for conceptual clarity, these conceptions overlap and can be interlinked. For instance, norms can act on individuals as both external pressures and as shaping intrinsic motivations (; ; ; ; ). calls for a view that combines stances, suggesting that norms are stronger when they exert both internal and external influences. echo this idea, suggesting that norms might be stronger when more community members agree with the norm. Social-norms theory would benefit from future research on these three distinct pathways, particularly on the ways in which norms vary in strength across them. Future research trajectories that aim to understand what influences the strength of social norms could build on earlier theoretical work carried out by on the return potential model (largely absent from the reviews). This model suggests that compliance with a norm does not necessarily result in approval: Overcompliance might generate negative reactions toward people who conform too strictly with the norm. Take the following example: A norm might exist that a worker should stay at the office until 5 p.m., so that noncompliers who leave at 3 p.m. are frowned upon. However, overcompliers (who, say, work until 8 p.m.) might also be frowned upon because they threaten the current equilibrium, pushing toward a normative model that might be difficult for other actors to follow (). As researchers try to understand what influences patterns of norms emergence, change, and maintenance, they might find inspiration in Jackson’s model as an example of how other group-related factors (such as the extent to which the group cares about the norm) will influence its strength.

What types of mechanisms are described in the reviews for how social norms come about, evolve, and dissipate?

Three stages of a norm’s life cycle surface as common themes across the reviews: emergence, when a norm comes into being; maintenance, when an established norm continues to influence behavior and practices over time; and change and disappearance, when a norm ceases to exist or to exert influence. Although there is some concordance on these three themes, we found debates and unanswered questions with regard to the mechanisms by which social norms move across these three stages.

We also found diverging language and understanding of the key stages in the life cycle of a social norm. Reviewers describe the key stages in different ways and break them down into different substages. We report the different conceptions included within these three categories in Table 4 and discuss the points of consensus and divergence about these three life stages in greater detail below.

Table 4.

Stages in the Life Cycle of a Norm

ClusterSubstageDescriptionEmergenceThe moment when a norm is instigated, when it comes into being as a candidate for a new normCreation“The process of presenting a new norm in a normative system is called norm creation” (, p. 7)Norm innovationWhen individuals “create new norms without any external interference” (, p. 7); also mentioned but not defined by ; ; Bicchieri & Muldoon (2014); and Norm ideation“Ideation is how an idea of behavior becomes a norm in the first place and filtering which ideas are accepted and rejected” (, p. 7))Process by which a norm starts to become recognized and accepted as a normNorm acquisition“How norms are acquired” (, p. 731)); also mentioned by ; ; and Norm assimilation“Norms assimilation is the process of joining and abiding by the rules and norms of a social group” (, p. 15)Norm acceptance“Norm acceptance is the process of conflict resolution where external enforcements on the agent vie against its internal desire” (, p. 15)Norm learning and social learning“Norm learning is the ability of learning from others and it is an active technique to complement and support the learning of individual” (, p. 10)“Individuals learn social norms via social learning whereby they observe others and enact behavior that others seem to approve of or endorse, while avoiding behavior that they see results in punishment” (, p. 9)“When an actor observes a customary action, the actor tries to make sense of this pattern. One inference that the actor might make is to infer that the action is customary because the action has provided benefits to others. . . . In a social learning process, it is not rewards from the group after performing the action that motivate the actor but the actor’s belief that the action will be rewarding in itself because other members of the group have previously been so rewarded” (, p. 34)“Process in which women gain information about the benefits and costs from the experiences of other women in their social network” (, p. 379)Can also involve “ritualized infant-caregiver interaction and mimicry” (, p. 732); also mentioned by and Norm adoptionWhen a norm is adopted by a significant number of people in a population (; ; )How norms emerge throughout a population or a groupSpreading and transmission“The process of distributing norms in a society or social group” (, p. 12); also mentioned by ; ; ; and DiffusionHow innovations are disseminated from a few individuals to a greater number of individuals in a population (; ; )MaintenanceHow norms become more established in a society or in individualsStabilization and crystallizationThe process by which norms become more stable in a culture (; )InstitutionalizationThe process by which norms become codified or encoded in institutions as formal rules in society (; ; )Internalization“A classic theory is that people follow the social patterns that they have internalized as personal norms . . . this means that objective social structures—regularities, sanctions and institutions—affect judgment and behavior via the personal norms that they inculcate” (, p. 5)“Internalization is an element of socialization whereby the actor learns to follow rules of behavior in situations that arouse impulses to transgress and there is no external surveillance or sanctions” (, p. 167)“When a norm in a society is widely accepted and becomes a routine task for the followers” (, p. 16)“Individuals internalize these persistent social pressures from external forces to internal preferences” (, p. 8)“Internalizing the values associated with a particular group and identifying with the attitudes and behaviors of other members of the group” (, p. 4); also mentioned by ; ; and Norms already exist but are relevant only in certain situationsNorm activationCognitive process by which an intention to act in a certain way becomes triggered in an individual’s mind, influencing the individual to act in accordance with the norm (; ; ).Norm detection“Norms detection is the process of updating an agent’s norms based on discovering a society’s potential norms through some detection mechanisms which rely on observing or interacting with other agents to infer the potential norms” (, p. 10)Long-term persistence of the normCultural continuity and stabilityThe extent to which norms persist across generations and are not altered ()ChangeNorms change from their original/prior formCreative mutationPart of cultural dynamics whereby systems of norms or values are both reproduced and altered ()Norms become more importantNorm bandwagons and cascades“Norm bandwagons occur when small shifts lead to large ones, as people join the ‘bandwagon’; norm cascades occur when there are rapid shifts in norms” (, p. 909)
“Includes wide norm acceptance specified by imitation, which attempts to socialize others to become followers” (, p. 16);
also mentioned by Norms become less importantDecrease in validity or diminishThe norm becomes less important to the majority of people; one can observe “the decrease in validity of a norm” (, p. 10)Norms disappearNorm removal“Norm removal is the ability of removing an obsolete norm and replacing it with a new norm which occurs when there is a conflict between the domain’s new norm and an internalized obsolete norm of an agent” (, p. 15)

Open in a separate window

Emergence

Fifteen reviews have discussed theories of norm emergence, examining why and how an action or social practice becomes accepted as a norm in some populations (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). A new norm can involve an action that was previously carried out by some people in the community but that was not considered a norm or a radically new way of acting and doing. Reviewers who discuss norm emergence conceptualize the transition from one substage to the next in different ways. Some theorize that behavior changes first and norms follow (; ; ). For instance, when smoking in restaurants and bars was first outlawed in Norway, people stopped smoking in public spaces before they began to believe that smoking in public was socially unacceptable. Other reviewers suggest instead the opposite: that norms change first, and behaviors follow, as it happens, for instance, when a certain “tipping point” is reached (see below; ; ). For example, there might be a norm that people should shake hands when they meet. At a time of an infectious epidemic, people might begin to prefer not doing so (as it would spread germs) and yet shake hands for fear of what others might say. These people would stop shaking hands only when they know that the norm has changed, that it is now acceptable not to shake hands.

Finally, reviewers have also considered the possibility of a process of mutual influence between the two levels: The more regular a behavior becomes in a population, the more individuals will believe there is a norm, and the more individuals believe that a norm exists, the more they will comply with it. As a result, the behavior becomes more common in the population (; ; ). This last interpretation seems to us the most reasonable one. The norm might change first in a given core group of people in a society (e.g., university students believing buying plastic bottles is inappropriate) and then be followed by a new behavior in that group (e.g., university students only buying glass bottles). Then, as their new actions become public, others might also adopt them (e.g., students’ families and friends buying plastic bottles), eventually bringing about further normative change in the larger society.

Maintenance

Seven reviews have included a discussion of norm maintenance and continuity: why and how norms tend to persist for long periods of time, how they persist after losing their original relevance or significance (; ; ), and what forces underlie these phenomena. Some discuss how norms persist because new group members learn and adopt them (). Others have called attention to the fact that norms are transmitted over generations and thus can persist even when the original group of norm followers has disappeared (; ; ; ). Finally, another set of reviews (; ; ) emphasizes norms’ relation to culture as the way to understand norm persistence. We thus note the recognition across reviews that norms persist and that norm continuity is a key feature of norms’ life cycle, although much of the literature does not address the processes and forces sustaining norms.

Change and dissipation

Reviews that examine when norms change tend to focus specifically on when norms shift naturally and quickly after long periods of stability. Three overlapping concepts are used to describe the process of quick normative change after long periods of persistence: tipping point, the specific moment when enough people hold attitudes against the existing norm and are ready to change; norm cascades, the process of norm change after a tipping point has been reached as more and more people start imitating those who are changing their behavior (enough people privately accept same-sex marriage that it becomes widely accepted in society; ; ; ; ; ); and punctuated equilibria, an overall description of the evolution of norms, in which a norm persists for a long period of time until it suddenly changes or disappears once it reaches a tipping point followed by a norm cascade (; ). We found only two reviewers who discuss how norms can change while not completely disappearing: Norms can be altered () or weakened (), suggesting the need for further inquiry into gradual norm change.

We suggest that these three life stages have potential conceptual overlaps. Norm change and norm emergence, for instance, are tightly linked: An emerging norm can potentially interfere with one that existed before, changing the latter. Likewise, an emerging norm can strengthen an existing one, facilitating the maintenance of this latter norm.

Mechanisms underlying norm dynamics

In addition to looking at these three life stages, some reviews discuss how norms move across stages, identifying several mechanisms that can impel norms to emerge, evolve, and dissipate. We mention five in particular: correction of misperceptions, structural changes, legal reforms, role models, and power dynamics.

Correction of misperceptions

Several reviewers suggest that people’s normative beliefs can change as they receive accurate information about what others in their group do and approve of (; ; ), specifically when the group’s beliefs are wrong, as they overestimate people who engage in and approve of a given harmful norm. This strategy, often referred to as “correcting misperceptions,” was historically adopted by health interventions that aimed to change harmful social norms by providing accurate information on what others in a given group did and approved of (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). The reviews discussed different sources of information, including interpersonal communication (; ; ), mass media (; ; ), informational campaigns (; ), small focus-group interventions (), observation of others (; ), and online platforms and video games (). Correcting misperceptions was, by far, the most commonly cited mechanism across the reviews. Two reviews also mentioned that strategies that increase the salience of positive norms can also work when there is no misperception to be corrected, that is, when people targeted by the intervention do not have any ideas of what others in their group are doing and approving of in relation to a given practice. Although studying this mechanism can yield important insights into how norms change, it needs to be integrated with other mechanisms explaining, for instance, how internalized norms can change.

Structural changes

Five reviews explore how variations in the social structure can influence the life trajectory of a norm. Background conditions, whether ecological (), historical (), or economic (), can affect the existence and evolution of norms and normative systems. find that perceived external threats (such as natural disasters, epidemics, or war) can increase the resilience of a norm as well as people’s readiness to sanction deviant individuals. argues that historical processes can affect what practices and values become normative at a given time in a given society to the extent that they give rise to “traditional” institutions and practices that demand compliance by virtue of their (real or perceived) traditional nature. Finally, changes in broader economic structures and institutions can influence people’s actions () by changing the economic implications of violating or complying with a norm, that is, by altering the costs and benefits of compliance () or more generally by affecting the nature of social interactions and hierarchies. One reviewer (), for instance, discusses how changes in both economic and social structures were necessary for feuding norms to dissipate. References to technological changes were absent from the reviewed literature, indicating important opportunities for future research.

Legal reforms

Two reviews suggest that legal reforms change social norms because they change what people believe to be approved or valued in their society (partly conflating this mechanism with the one on “correction of misperceptions”; ; ). However, as notes, the coercive function of a law can act as an enforcement mechanism shaping new external obligations. Legal reforms are not always effective in changing the norm: Social norms and legal rules are not always aligned and can, in fact, have contradictory effects () or act as substitutes for one another (; ). At times, changing legal rules might not be effective in changing social norms (), as they might, for instance, force practices to go underground, in effect strengthening them by making them undetectable to the eyes of those would disapprove of it (which would eventually contribute to changing the norm). The scope exists for future research to explore under what circumstances legal reforms do change social norms, including in states that do not have strong control over their territory.

Role models

Nine reviewers highlight the role that influential individuals can play in inducing others to change their behavior, referring to them as leaders (), norm entrepreneurs (; ; ), opinion leaders (; ; ), social referents (; ), or role models (). These individuals exert social influence and persuasion through emotions, social attachment, personal connections, institutionally or socially conferred authority, or ease of personal identification. Examples of potentially influential individuals include authority figures such as religious leaders or village elders (; ), individuals holding a special status in society (), and peers or friends (; ). They can influence norm dynamics at all stages of a norm’s life cycle, from facilitating the diffusion, transmission, and spreading of norms to encouraging others to adopt a new norm or abandon an existing one, instigating norm cascades.

Power dynamics

Five reviews discuss how power relations can affect the emergence or dissipation of social norms, as happens when, for instance, the diffusion of a new norm in a group encounters active resistance from some powerful members (; ; ; ; ). argue that power is central to understanding norm compliance. They divide the literature into theories that favor “power explanations” (norms take hold in a top-down manner through formal institutions and powerholders) and those that favor “historical explanations” (norms emerge and change from the bottom up naturally through time). The study of how power dynamics affect norms requires understanding the ways in which groups and individuals can affect norm dynamics on the basis of the place they occupy in the social hierarchy (; ; ) and which specific individuals or groups have the ability to enforce or resist the adoption of a norm (), as, for instance, in the case of a trade union dominated by people of a given race or gender who carry out exclusionary strategies to maintain their privileged position in the labor force. When powerful groups or individuals have an important role to play in the transformation of norms, collective action and social movements must offset established hierarchies and powerful groups () or involve them strategically in the movement for social improvement (; ).

Looking at these mechanisms synoptically allows greater critical awareness of the appropriateness of existing methods to shift harmful norms. The traditional social norms approach has largely focused on correcting misperceptions (; ). These health interventions aim to increase people’s awareness that only a low percentage of people engages in a harmful practice. These interventions (common across U.S. university campuses) spread messages such as “At Stanford University, 9 students out of 10 do not drink alcohol on Saturday night” or “93% of men living in Paris agree that only cowards hit women.” However, such an approach works only when there is widespread (mostly tacit) support to changing the norm, and for this reason it has recently been criticized as a “narrow” approach to social norms change (). New approaches are now integrating strategies that work with core groups of populations to change their attitudes first and equip them with skills and knowledge to become agents of change in their community, with effective results in changing social norms by working with powerholders, role models, and policymakers (; ). As practitioners and scholars collaborate further to bridge social-norms theory and practice, they might want to take into account the five mechanisms we identified and test how they can be integrated into effective programs.

What categories of agents are identified in the reviews as relevant in the study of social norms?

Several reviews mention the “reference group” (defined below) as an important element of social-norms theory (; ; ; ; ; ). Those that did not mention the concept explicitly still discuss how different social norms are created and reproduced within social groups, sustaining practices that are ritualized as symbols of group membership in ways that affect people’s self-understanding (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). In this section, we discuss the categories of groups that are relevant to understanding social norms, as revealed by our analysis.

In broad terms, reference group refers to the relevant others whose behavior and (dis)approval matter in sustaining the norm. Different social norms can have different reference groups (smoking might be a norm in a group of adolescents but not in the adolescents’ families), and the same norm can change across different groups (tipping is prescribed in the United States but proscribed in Japan; ; ).

Across the reviews, and even within reviews, we found several—sometimes conflicting—uses of the term reference group. The confusion is problematic because many methods for studying norms and norm change involve identifying the reference group related to a norm () but in practice might be measuring different groups of people. We identified three categories of people that are key to understanding how social norms are sustained: norm targets, the people who comply with the norm; norm drivers, the people who exert influence over the norm’s life cycle; and norm beneficiaries and victims, the people who are affected by the social norm, including when they are neither actors nor influencers (see Table 5).

Table 5.

Actor Categories Mentioned in the Reviews

Category and subcategoryDescriptionReferencesExampleTargets SubjectsThe group of people the norm applies to; the individuals who are supposed to follow the norm; ; ; ; ; and Subway passengers in the norm: “You should let people off the subway before going on” Members of a group I want to belong toThe group of people that are members of social groups one wants to be a part of or identifies with; ; Bicchieri & Muldoon (2014); ; ; ; ; ; and Popular students in the norm: “Popular students get drunk when partying”Norm drivers EnforcersPeople who apply sanctions, react to violations of a norm, or reward compliance
In the case of institutionalized norms, the state might be the enforcer; in other cases it can be community members or even the entire population; ; ; ; ; ; ; and Parents punishing their children for not complying with the norm that “children should obey to their parents” Social influencersPeople who exert social influence on individuals (other than sanctions), motivating them to comply with the norm; ; ; ; ; and Peers, family, role models, teachers Norm leadersIndividuals with the ability to influence or convince others to adopt a new norm or change their behavior (also called opinion leaders, norm entrepreneurs, or change agents); ; ; ; ; and Religious leaders calling for an end to child marriage Norm followersMajority of the population that follows norm leaders to update their beliefs, evaluations, or behaviors; ; ; and People who buy a smartphone because it is now a popular trend Powerful groupsGroups that have the ability to direct/control norm dynamics, such as introducing a new norm or resisting norm change and Religious groups that oppose women’s use of contraceptionBeneficiaries and victims BeneficiariesThe people who benefit from the norm or its consequences Can include the entire population in the case of norms of cooperation or a specific group and Nonsmokers benefitting from a norm that “smokers should not smoke in public places” VictimsThose negatively affected by a normGirls who do not want to get married in the norm that “girls should get married soon after puberty”

Open in a separate window

Norm targets

At times, the term reference group is used to define the people to whom the norm applies (; ; ; ; ), the subjects, or—using a term coined by —the targets of the norm (; ; ). Take, for instance, a group of people living in a condominium. They hear domestic violence but decide not to intervene because there is a norm that “people in this neighborhood do not intervene in other family’s business.” The norm thus targets “people in this neighborhood.” Targets can be either members of specific groups or social categories (e.g., pedestrians, adolescent girls, chief executive officers). Two reviews mention that people can spontaneously make themselves targets of a norm, motivated by the desire to be associated with specific social categories (; ).

Norm drivers

At other times, the term reference group alludes to the group of people whose influence contributes to maintaining a given norm. Norm drivers would be, for instance, a group of adolescents exerting pressure on a peer to make him comply with a smoking norm (; ; ). These norm drivers do not necessarily comply with the norm themselves, but their opinions and actions matter in shaping people’s beliefs about what important others do and approve of. Several types of norm drivers exist (see Table 5). Enforcers actively encourage conformity with the existing status quo and contribute to maintaining a social norm in place. Leaders, by contrast, set norm change in motion (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ), either because they are particularly influential () or because they are more willing to bear the costs of violating a norm (). Finally, norm followers are those who change their actions to comply with a new, emerging norm after norm leaders do or after a large proportion of the population do; they are key to moving a population into a new normative equilibrium in which a large majority complies with the new norm (; ; ; ; ).

Norm beneficiaries and victims

No reviews use the term reference group to refer to those who are affected by a norm—those who here we call norm beneficiaries and victims. We believe this third category to be important enough to deserve separate recognition. Norm beneficiaries are those who gain from the existing normative equilibrium. In the domestic-violence example offered earlier, a perpetrator of domestic violence benefits from a norm that “people in this neighborhood do not intervene in other family’s business.” Norm victims are those who lose from the existing normative equilibrium (as the victim of domestic violence above). The distinction between beneficiaries and victims is not always straightforward and will sometimes depend on the observer’s judgment. An adolescent might say they “benefit” from binge drinking in that they receive the approval of their peers, thus improving their sense of belonging, whereas a public-health policymaker might believe that that same adolescent is harmed by the norm.

Although for conceptual clarity we have presented three separate categories (norm targets, norm drivers, and norm beneficiaries/victims), in the real world they often overlap. Targets can influence others through their compliance as well as benefitting or being harmed by the norm. Take, for instance, the norm that prescribes punctuality in an organization. In this case, all three groups are the same: norm targets (who comply with the norm by arriving on time for meetings) are also norm drivers (as they disapprove or complain about violators) and norm beneficiaries (as by complying with the norm they save time and ensure that their work can be achieved more effectively). A norm that adolescent girls should get married soon after puberty offers instead an example in which only some of the three groups overlap. Norm targets are the adolescent girls, who must get married soon after puberty, but (especially when they do not want to marry young) they are also norm victims. Parents, community members, and traditional leaders could be both norm drivers and norm beneficiaries ().

Conclusion

We set out in this article to investigate how multidisciplinary reviews of social-norms theory organized this large body of literature. Four thematic areas of investigation emerged as we explored the articles that made it through our screening. The first related to the nature of social norms; here, we identified areas of debate and consensus, especially with regard to whether social norms are individual or collective constructs (see Tables 2 and and3).3). We suggested that these two approaches might be useful to different scholars and practitioners (e.g., the former to those working on behavioral change in global health and international development and the latter to historians and sociologists). The second area of investigation related to the pathways through which norms influence people’s actions. Areas of consensus and debate related to whether one or multiple pathways lead from norms to action, with our preference for the latter explanation. The pathways in the reviews naturally clustered into three categories: norms offering value-neutral information, norms creating external obligations, and norms becoming internal obligations. The third area related to the life stages of social norms: how they emerge, survive, and dissipate. We identified several substages across the reviews (see Table 4) and uncovered five key mechanisms that facilitate the movement of a norm across these life stages: correction of misperceptions, structural changes, legal reforms, role models, and power dynamics. We suggested that efforts to change social norms should look at how these five together interact and overlap rather than investing time and resources into only one of them.

Finally, the fourth and last area of investigation related to the groups relevant to the study of social norms. We discussed the role that norm targets, norm drivers, and norm beneficiaries/victims have to play. Even though the last of these categories was not found in the reviews, we argued for its inclusion in further work on social norms. Overall, we also found that reviews of the social-norms literature could benefit from a closer engagement with social theory and related literature in the social sciences. Future cross-disciplinary reviews of social-norms theory might cover bordering theoretical space, engaging with the relation between norms theory and, for instance, theories looking at social capital (), social dominance (), and intersectional inequalities based on gender, class, or race (). Very few of the reviews included in this study tried to accomplish such a task. is one exception, as his work looked at how norms intersect with power relations in ways that sustain or undermine a given social order. is the only other exception; his work partly looks at how acculturation and involvement in social or religious movements can change social norms.

From the current study, two important lines of enquiry emerge as trajectories for future research. First, future research could investigate how the different mechanisms underlying social norms dynamics operate at different stages in the life of a norm. Future theoretical and empirical studies could map out what specific mechanisms are relevant to particular stages and substages in the life cycle of a norm. Here, engagement with theories of diffusion (), acculturation (), and structural ritualization () might be particularly fruitful. Second, future research could increase our understanding of the relations and transitions between the three normative pathways we identified (providing information, creating external obligations, and becoming internal obligations). Research questions along this line of inquiry would include how and when norms are internalized, how and when changes in individuals’ preferences weaken social norms, and how people navigate conflicting influences from different normative pathways. Here, action identification theory () and social identity theory () might assist a researcher looking at the further integration of social norms and social theory. As empirical and theoretical work on social norms advances into its next phase of investigation, we hope for greater cross-disciplinary work to extend and improve our understanding of the rules that bind us, expanding what may be one of the oldest research trajectories in the history of human thought.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Leah Kenny and Susan Igras for reviewing and commenting on a prepublication draft of the manuscript.

Notes

1.We are grateful for this example to Molly Melching of the nongovernmental organization Tostan, who, building on their observations during the recent Ebola epidemics in West Africa, uses this example in their training program.

Footnotes

Action Editor: Laura A. King served as action editor for this article.

ORCID iD: Beniamino Cislaghi

What is the process through which people get to understand the social norms
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6296-4644

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.

References

Anderson E. (2000). Beyond homo economicus: New developments in theories of social norms. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 29, 170–200. [Google Scholar]

Anderson J. E., Dunning D. (2014). Behavioral norms: Variants and their identification. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 721–738. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12146 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Bandura A., Freeman W., Lightsey R. (1999). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 13, 158–166. [Google Scholar]

Bell D. C., Cox M. L. (2015). Social norms: Do we love norms too much? Journal of Family Theory & Review, 7, 28–46. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Berkowitz A. (2002). Applications of social norms theory to other health and social justice issues. In Perkins H. W. (Ed.), The social norms approach to preventing school and college age substance abuse: A handbook for educators, counselors, clinicians (pp. 259–279). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. [Google Scholar]

Berkowitz A. (2004). An overview of the social norms approach. In Lederman L. C., Stewart L. P. (Eds.), Changing the culture of college drinking: A socially situated health communication campaign (pp. 187–208). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. [Google Scholar]

Bicchieri C. (2006). The grammar of society: The nature and dynamics of social norms. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]

Bicchieri C., Muldoon R., Sontuoso A. (2011). Social norms. In Zalta E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Winter 2018. ed.). Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2018/entries/social-norms/.

Bingenheimer J. B. (2019). Veering from a narrow path: The second decade of social norms research. Journal of Adolescent Health, 64(4), S1–S3. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.01.012 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Boyd R., Richerson P. J. (1994). The evolution of norms: An anthropological view. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics (JITE)/Zeitschrift für die gesamte Staatswissenschaft, 150, 72–87. [Google Scholar]

Boytsun A., Deloof M., Matthyssens P. (2011). Social norms, social cohesion, and corporate governance. Corporate Governance: An International Review, 19, 41–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8683.2010.00829.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Burke M. A., Young P. H. (2011). Social norms. In Benhabib J., Bisin A., Jackson M. O. (Eds.), Handbook of social economics (Vol. 1, pp. 311–338). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland. [Google Scholar]

Butler J. (2004). Undoing gender. New York, NY: Routledge. [Google Scholar]

Chung A., Rimal R. N. (2016). Social norms: A review. Review of Communication Research, 4, 1–28. [Google Scholar]

Cialdini R. B., Reno R. R., Kallgren C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1015–1026. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Cialdini R. B., Trost M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity and compliance. In Gilbert D. T., Fiske S. T., Lindzey G. (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 151–192). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]

Cislaghi B., Denny E. K., Cisse M., Gueye P., Shrestha B., Shrestha P. N., . . . Clark C. J. (2019). Changing social norms: The importance of “organized diffusion” for scaling up community health promotion and women empowerment interventions. Prevention Science, 20, 936–946. doi: 10.1007/s11121-019-00998-3 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Cislaghi B., Heise L. (2018. a). Four avenues of normative influence. Health Psychology, 37, 562–573. doi: 10.1037/hea0000618 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Cislaghi B., Heise L. (2019). Using social norms theory for health promotion in low-income countries. Health Promotion International, 34, 616–623. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Coleman J. S. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]

Dalton R. J. (2008). Citizenship norms and the expansion of political participation. Political Studies, 56, 76–98. [Google Scholar]

Dannals J. E., Miller D. T. (2017). Social norms in organizations. In Hitt M. A. (Ed.), Oxford research encyclopedia of business and management. doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190224851.013.139. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

de Groot J. I. M., Schuitema G. (2012). How to make the unpopular popular? Policy characteristics, social norms and the acceptability of environmental policies. Environmental Science & Policy, 19–20, 100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.envsci.2012.03.004 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Deutsch M., Gerard H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Durkheim É. (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. [Google Scholar]

Etzioni A. (2000). Social norms: Internalization, persuasion, and history. Law & Society Review, 34, 157–178. [Google Scholar]

Fleming P. J., Agnew-Brune C. (2015). Current trends in the study of gender norms and health behaviors. Current Opinion in Psychology, 5, 72–77. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.05.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Gibbs J. P. (1965). Norms: The problem of definition and classification. American Journal of Sociology, 70, 586–594. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Goldstein N. J., Mortensen C. R. (2012). Social norms: A how-to (and how-not-to) guide. In Kenrick D. T., Goldstein N. J., Braver S. L. (Eds.), Six degrees of social influence: Science, application, and the psychology of Robert Cialdini (pp. 68–78). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]

Grant R. W. (1988). Locke’s political anthropology and Lockean individualism. The Journal of Politics, 50, 42–63. doi: 10.2307/2131040 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Gregory G. D., Munch J. M. (1997). Cultural values in international advertising: An examination of familial norms and roles in Mexico. Psychology & Marketing, 14, 99–119. [Google Scholar]

Hecter M., Opp K.-D. (Eds.). (2001). Social norms. New York, NY: Russel Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]

Heiphetz L., Spelke E. S., Harris P. L., Banaji M. R. (2014). What do different beliefs tell us? An examination of factual, opinion-based, and religious beliefs. Cognitive Development, 30, 15–29. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Hogg M. A. (2016). Social identity theory. In McKeown S., Haji R., Ferguson N. (Eds.), Understanding peace and conflict through social identity theory (pp. 3–17). New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]

Hong H., Kacperczyk M. (2009). The price of sin: The effects of social norms on markets. Journal of Financial Economics, 93, 15–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jfineco.2008.09.001 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Hume D. (1978). A treatise of human nature (Selby-Bigge L. A., Ed.) (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1739) [Google Scholar]

Jackson J. (1966). A conceptual and measurement model for norms and roles. The Pacific Sociological Review, 9, 35–47. [Google Scholar]

Jensen Arnett J. (2015). Socialization in emerging adulthood: From the family to the wider world, from socialization to self-socialization. In Grusec J. E., Hastings P. D. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 208–231). New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]

Kahneman D., Miller D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153. [Google Scholar]

Keyt D., Miller F. D. (1993). A companion to Aristotle’s “Politics.” Ethics, 103, 387–389. [Google Scholar]

Knottnerus J. D. (1997). The theory of structural ritualization. In Advances in group processes (Vol. 14, pp. 257–280). Bingley, England: Emerald Publishing. [Google Scholar]

Lapinski M. K., Rimal R. N. (2005). An explication of social norms. Communication Theory, 15, 127–147. [Google Scholar]

Lin N., Cook K. S., Burt R. S. (2001). Social capital: Theory and research. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers. [Google Scholar]

Loch K. D., Straub D. W., Kamel S. (2003). Diffusing the Internet in the Arab world: The role of social norms and technological culturation. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, 50, 45–63. [Google Scholar]

Mackie G., Moneti F., Shakya H., Denny E. (2015). What are social norms? How are they measured? New York, NY: UNICEF. [Google Scholar]

Mahmoud M. A., Ahmad M. S., Yusoff M. Z. M., Mustapha A. (2014). A review of norms and normative multiagent systems. The Scientific World Journal, 2014, Article 684587. doi: 10.1155/2014/684587 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

McPherson M., Smith-Lovin L., Cook J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415–444. [Google Scholar]

Merton R. K., Merton R. C. (1968). Social theory and social structure. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. [Google Scholar]

Miller D. T., Prentice D. A. (1994). Collective errors and errors about the collective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 541–550. [Google Scholar]

Morris M. W., Hong Y.-y., Chiu C.-y., Liu Z. (2015). Normology: Integrating insights about social norms to understand cultural dynamics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.03.001 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Nichols S. (2002). On the genealogy of norms: A case for the role of emotion in cultural evolution. Philosophy of Science, 69, 234–255. [Google Scholar]

Norenzayan A., Shariff A. F., Gervais W. M., Willard A. K., McNamara R. A., Slingerland E., Henrich J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 39, Article e1. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14001356 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Ostrom E. (2014). Collective action and the evolution of social norms. Journal of Natural Resources Policy Research, 6, 235–252. doi: 10.1080/19390459.2014.935173 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Petty R. E., Brinol P. (2010). Attitude change. In Baumeister R. F., Finkel E. J. (Eds.), Advanced social psychology: The state of the science (pp. 217–259). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]

Popitz H. (2017). Social norms. Genocide Studies and Prevention: An International Journal, 11(2), 3–12. [Google Scholar]

Posner E. A. (2009). Law and social norms. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]

Pulerwitz J., Blum R., Cislaghi B., Costenbader E., Harper C., Heise L., . . . Lundgren R. (2019). Proposing a conceptual framework to address social norms that influence adolescent sexual and reproductive health. Journal of Adolescent Health, 64(4 Suppl.), S7–S9. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.01.014 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Reid A. E., Cialdini R. B., Aiken L. S. (2010). Social norms and health behaviour. In Steptoe A. (Ed.), Handbook of behavioral medicine: Methods and applications (pp. 263–274). New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]

Samuels G. M., Ross-Sheriff F. (2008). Identity, oppression, and power: Feminisms and intersectionality theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. [Google Scholar]

Seidman S. (2016). Contested knowledge: Social theory today. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]

Sheeran P., Maki A., Montanaro E., Avishai-Yitshak A., Bryan A., Klein W. M., . . . Rothman A. J. (2016). The impact of changing attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy on health-related intentions and behavior: A meta-analysis

What is it called when you follow social norms?

Social norms are the accepted standards of behavior of social groups. These groups range from friendship and workgroups to nation-states. behavior which fulfills these norms is called conformity, and most of the time roles and norms are powerful ways of understanding and predicting what people will do.

How are social norms learned?

Social norms are communicated by what people do and say in their everyday lives, which can be indirect (e.g., inferring norms from others' behaviors) but also direct (e.g., intentionally talking about what is and is not normative of the group; Hogg & Reid, 2006).

What is the process of socialization?

Socialization is a learning process that begins shortly after birth. Early childhood is the period of the most intense and the most crucial socialization. It is then that we acquire language and learn the fundamentals of our culture. It is also when much of our personality takes shape.

What is social norms in understanding the self?

Social norms are typically defined as “rules and standards that are understood by members of a group, and that guide or constrain social behaviors without the force of law” (Cialdini and Trost, 1998, p. 152), and often relate to a perceived social pressure to engage or not engage in specific behaviors (Ajzen, 1991).