Stranger anxiety in infants tends to be influenced by which of the following?

Coincidence or cascade? The temporal relation between locomotor behaviors and the emergence of stranger anxiety

Author links open overlay panelRebecca J.BrandPersonEnvelopeKellyEscobarAriana M.Patrick

Show
Show moreNavigate Down

ShareShare

Cited ByCite

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101423Get rights and content

Abstract

Two infant milestones, self-propelled locomotion and stranger anxiety, tend to emerge at a similar age in development. An adaptive relation may exist in which the onset of one influences the development of the other in individual children. We examine whether these milestones systematically co-occur and whether one reliably precedes the other. In the current study, 104 parents completed weekly online surveys between 6 and 12 months, noting milestones as they occurred. Onset ages for locomotor behaviors were correlated with onset of stranger anxiety over and above a milestone in a third domain, namely consonant-vowel babbling. These data suggest that infants’ earliest locomotor behaviors may play a role in galvanizing the development of stranger anxiety.

Introduction

In the first year of life, human development proceeds at a furious pace. Infants progress from squirming and mewing to toddling and shouting in mere months, passing through a flurry of motor, cognitive, social, and emotional milestones along the way. Two of these milestones tend to occur around the same age (7–8 months): independent locomotion (prototypically in the form of hands-and-knees crawling) and a wariness of strangers (often called stranger anxiety)1 . Given the sheer number of milestones infants achieve in this short time, the co-occurrence of two could be coincidental (see Bertenthal, Campos, & Barrett, 1984). However, it seems adaptive for locomotion and stranger anxiety to go hand-in-hand in individual children (Bowlby, 1969; Bretherton, 1992). For instance, perhaps stranger anxiety appears first and then hastens locomotor development in order to help infants regulate the distance from their parents. Equally plausible, locomotion might emerge and then trigger in infants a new awareness of the social environment and a coinciding wariness of strangers (Campos et al., 2000; Mahler, Pine, & Bergman, 1975; Thelen, 2000). The current paper examines whether these milestones systematically co-occur in individual infants, and whether one milestone reliably precedes the other in development.

The beginning of crawling is a much-celebrated milestone for parents. A majority of infants in the United States crawl on hands-and-knees prior to walking, with average age of onset around 8 months (Adolph et al., 2015; Adolph, Vereijken, & Denny, 1998; Capute, Shapiro, Palmer, Ross, & Wachtel, 1985; Gesell, 1928). In addition, other, less-efficient forms of locomotion often emerge before hands-and-knees crawling. For instance, Adolph et al. (1998) found that about half of infants in their sample used some form of “belly” crawl before developing the typical hands-and-knees gait. Other infants “may log roll from place to place, or pivot in circles” (Adolph & Franchak, 2017, p. 4). In other words, according to these authors, “individual infants find different ways to solve the problem of moving” (4).

A clear change in infants’ response to strangers is also typically apparent around the same time. Young infants generally accept proximity and contact by unknown adults. Around eight months, however, infants show a marked increase in fussy and avoidant behavior toward unfamiliar adults, referred to as stranger anxiety, stranger fear, or stranger wariness (Boyer & Bergstrom, 2011; Bronson, 1972; Freedman, 1961; Gaensbauer, Emde, & Campos, 1976; Spitz, 1965; Sroufe, 1977). Although “wariness” may be the most apt term, encompassing everything from staring to averted gaze to frank distress and crying (LoBue and Adolph, 2019; Sroufe, 1977; Waters, Matas, & Sroufe, 1975), we chose to use the classic term “stranger anxiety” as we felt it might be most familiar to parent participants.

Stranger anxiety may serve as an adaptive countermeasure to infants’ locomotion, helping infants maintain a balance between the urge to explore the environment and the need to feel safe and secure (Campos et al., 2000; Freedman, 1974/2016Freedman, /, 2016Freedman, 1974/2016; Mahler et al., 1975). As Bretherton (1985), p.8) put it, “a system that ensures that a child’s explorations do not take it too far from a protective figure can … plausibly be viewed as having survival value.” According to this thinking, the appearance of stranger anxiety and locomotion together seems likely to be functional, rather than coincidental. Perhaps stranger anxiety develops first as part of the unfolding attachment system and subsequently galvanizes infants to locomote in order to regulate their social environment. In support of this possibility, infants in the strange situation paradigm show attenuated fear reactions when they are able to approach the familiar caregiver (e.g. Ainsworth & Bell, 1970; Kotelchuck, Zelazo, Kagan, & Spelke, 1975).

On the other hand, perhaps infants’ desires to explore the environment encourage them to locomote, and subsequently this spurs the onset of stranger anxiety. This possibility seems especially plausible given the evidence that the onset of crawling predicts a huge variety of perceptual, cognitive, emotional, and social developments (e.g., Adolph, Kretch, & LoBue, 2014; Brand, Escobar, Baranès, & Albu, 2015; Campos, Kermoian, & Zumbahlen, 1992; Cicchino & Rakison, 2008; Higgins, Campos, & Kermoian, 1996; Kermoian & Campos, 1988; Uchiyama et al., 2008; see Campos et al., 2000, for a review). Campos et al. (2000) found that locomotor infants were more responsive to maternal separations and paid more attention to distal events than pre-locomotor infants. One explanation for these changes may be that experience locomoting encourages a broader attention to the physical and social environment (Campos et al., 2000; Thelen, 2000; Uchiyama et al., 2008).

If either locomotion or stranger anxiety is systematically prompting the development of the other, we would expect the causal factor to appear reliably first. In addition, if locomotion and stranger anxiety unfold according to either of the above proposals, we would expect the ages of milestone achievement to be correlated across a sample of children. If such a relation is specific to these two milestones, it should persist over and above relations with a third variable also appearing at this age, such as consonant-vowel babbling (Gillis, Schauwers, & Govaerts, 2002). To address these questions, we surveyed parents on the emergence of a number of milestones, including locomotion, stranger anxiety, and babbling2 .

In a large pilot study, we surveyed 304 parents of infants between 6 and 12 months and asked them to approximate the dates of onset of hands-and-knees crawling (specified as traveling two or more body lengths on hands and knees) and stranger anxiety (defined as refusing to go to an unfamiliar adult). Reporting was retrospective, but across a relatively short delay (usually 1–2 months). The pilot data suggested that the ages of the two milestones were correlated across infants. We also found that on average, stranger anxiety appeared before crawling, but a substantial minority of infants developed crawling before stranger anxiety.

To gather milestone data in a more precise and accurate way, in the current study we followed parents prospectively, starting prior to the achievement of the key milestones, and administered a weekly survey designed to identify milestones as they occurred. Given the previously-reported transformative effects of self-produced locomotion, we were somewhat surprised to find the pilot work suggesting a tendency for stranger anxiety to occur before crawling. We considered whether perhaps pre-crawling locomotor attempts might be enough to trigger a broader awareness of the social environment. By the time infants are skilled crawlers, they may have been moving in other ways for some time (Adolph et al., 1998). Thus, in the current study, in addition to hands-and-knees crawling, we also assess other forms of locomotion. To accomplish this, we asked parents about early locomotor forms such as belly or “commando” crawling, as well as rolling or scooting, provided they were seen as attempts to “get across the room” (see below).

Section snippets

Participants

A total of 104 parents of infants (48 males, 56 females) completed one or more weekly surveys regarding their infants’ milestones. Parents were invited to begin any time after their infant turned six months old and were asked to continue each week until the first birthday, or until their infants reached all of the key milestones. However, some parents stopped completing surveys before their child reached all the milestones or the first birthday. Parents completed an average of 7.86 weekly

Discussion

We examined whether the onset of locomotion and stranger anxiety were systematically related across infants, and whether one milestone reliably occurred before the other. We found that the parent-reported ages for locomotor onset and stranger anxiety were correlated, and this relation remained significant after controlling for babbling age. Regarding order of emergence, infants developed some form of locomotion significantly earlier than stranger anxiety, with more than two-thirds of infants

Author statement

The first author (Brand) conceived of the initial research question, and took the lead role in manuscript preparation and data analysis.

Escobar played a major role in planning and executing data collection, and took a major role in manuscript preparation.

Patrick played a major role in planning and executing data collection, and took a secondary role in manuscript preparation.

Acknowledgements

The authors want to thank Dr. Gwen Broude for planting the seed for this paper 25 years ago in a psychology class at Vassar College. We also thank Nicole Blekhter and Erica Ferrara for assistance in data collection and coding and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful feedback. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References (44)

  • K.A. Thomas et al.

    Validity and Mechanical Turk: An assessment of exclusion methods and interactive experiments

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2017)

  • H.L. Rheingold et al.

    Fear of the stranger: A critical examination

    Advances in child development and behavior

    (1974)

  • I.P. Kan et al.

    Do imposters threaten data quality? An examination of worker misrepresentation and downstream consequences in Amazon’s Mechanical Turk workforce

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2018)

  • S. Houwen et al.

    The interrelationships between motor, cognitive, and language development in children with and without intellectual and developmental disabilities

    Research in Developmental Disabilities

    (2016)

  • M.A. Gartstein et al.

    Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire

    Infant Behavior & Development

    (2003)

  • P. Boyer et al.

    Threat-detection in child development: An evolutionary perspective

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2011)

  • K.E. Adolph et al.

    The development of motor behavior

    Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science

    (2017)

  • K.E. Adolph et al.

    Handbook of intra-individual variability across the lifespan

    (2015)

  • K.E. Adolph et al.

    Fear of heights in infants?

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2014)

  • K.E. Adolph et al.

    Learning to crawl

    Child Development

    (1998)

  • K. Adolph et al.

    What is the shape of developmental change?

    Psychological Review

    (2008)

  • M.D.S. Ainsworth et al.

    Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation

    Child Development

    (1970)

  • B.I. Bertenthal et al.

    Self-produced locomotion

    Continuities and discontinuities in development

    (1984)

  • J. Bowlby

    Attachment and loss

    (1969)

  • R.J. Brand et al.

    Crawling predicts infants’ understanding of agents’ navigation of obstacles

    Infancy

    (2015)

  • I. Bretherton

    Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1985)

  • I. Bretherton

    The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth

    Developmental Psychology

    (1992)

  • G.W. Bronson

    Infants’ reactions to unfamiliar persons and novel objects

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1972)

  • J.J. Campos et al.

    Travel broadens the mind

    Infancy

    (2000)

  • J.J. Campos et al.

    Socioemotional transformations in the family system following infant crawling onset

    New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development

    (1992)

  • A.J. Capute et al.

    Normal gross motor development: The influences of race, sex and socio-economic status

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology

    (1985)

  • J.B. Cicchino et al.

    Producing and processing self-propelled motion in infancy

    Developmental Psychology

    (2008)

  • Navigate DownView more references

    Cited by (1)

    • The human fear paradox: Affective origins of cooperative care

      2022, Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    • Research article

      Sleep duration associates with moderate-to-vigorous intensity physical activity and body fat in 1- to 3-year-old children

      Infant Behavior and Development, Volume 58, 2020, Article 101392

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Sleep during early childhood is important for many developmental outcomes and shows promise as an important correlate of both obesity risk and physical activity behaviors. This was a cross-sectional study concerning the relationships between sleep and moderate- to – vigorous intensity physical activity and body fat percentage in a sample of 1- to 3-year-old children (N = 50; ages 27.512 ± 10.363 months). Sleep was measured with a caregiver questionnaire. Sedentary time, light, moderate, vigorous, and moderate- to – vigorous intensity physical activity were measured with Actigraph wT3x-BT accelerometers. Body fat was measured using Air Displacement Plethysmography with the BodPod Pediatric Option. Moderate- to – vigorous intensity physical activity and body fat percentage both associated with sleep duration, controlling for age and accelerometer wear time. These factors combined explained 54.3 % of the variance in sleep duration present in the sample. These results suggest the presence of relationships between sleep and physical activity and body composition constructs in this sample. Sleep may be an important variable in efforts to both promote early childhood physical activity and healthy body composition.

    • Research article

      Do early parental postnatal depression, attachment style and perceived social support affect neurodevelopmental outcomes of premature infants?

      Infant Behavior and Development, Volume 59, 2020, Article 101444

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      The birth of a premature infant is both a stressful event for both parents and associated with an increased rate of postnatal depression (PND). Additionally some mothers may have delayed feelings of attachment to their babies because of the medical procedures or possible medical complications. Social support is known as an important factor for well-being in the postnatal period. However there is scarce data about these factors for fathers. We aimed to identify the impact of parental PND, attachment style and social support on premature infant development considering the prematurity degree and risk groups.

      This prospective study was conducted by including 96 infants who were born preterm. Mothers and fathers were given Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS), Adult Attachment Style Scale (AASS), and Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) to fill out when their infants’ corrected age was 3 months. The developmental evaluation was conducted with Bayley III at the corrected 6 months and 18 months of age.

      Postnatal depression scores were more in mothers than fathers, the rates of secure attachment and social support were similar between mothers and fathers. Factors associated with the neurodevelopmental outcomes including prematurity degree and risk groups, EPDS, AASS and MSPSS scores were analyzed for both parents. In multivariate analysis, fathers’ depression scores were inversely associated with cognitive development (p = 0.030, R2 = 0.080, B=-0.283) and mothers’ anxious/ambivalent attachment style was inversely associated with language development (p = 0.011, R2 = 0.108, B=-0.329) at the age of corrected 6 months old.

      Our findings underscore that the efforts to improve developmental outcomes of premature infants should include parental well-being taking into account new fathers’ depressive symptomatology and maternal anxious/ambivalent attachment.

    • Research article

      Does preterm birth affect child temperament? A meta-analytic study

      Infant Behavior and Development, Volume 58, 2020, Article 101417

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      The present meta-analytic study was conducted to examine differences in temperament between preterm and full-term children, considering behavior style and psychobiological approaches. Moreover, we explored the potential moderators of the associations between prematurity and temperament. A systematic literature search was performed on PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, and CINAHL. Twenty-two studies were analyzed. Preterm children showed a higher Activity level as well as lower Attentional Focusing and Attention Span/Persistence, in comparison with their full-term counterparts. Extremely preterm children showed a higher Activity level than other preterm levels. These findings support the conclusion that preterm children present with a less regulated temperament relative to those born full term.

    • Research article

      Neuroimaging the sleeping brain: Insight on memory functioning in infants and toddlers

      Infant Behavior and Development, Volume 58, 2020, Article 101427

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Episodic memory, or the ability to remember past events with specific detail, is central to the human experience and is related to learning and adaptive functioning in a variety of domains. In typically developing children, episodic memory emerges during infancy and improves during early childhood and beyond. Developmental processes within the hippocampus are hypothesized to be primarily responsible for both the early emergence and persistence of episodic memory in late infancy and early childhood. However, these hypotheses are based on non-human models. In-vivo investigations in early human development of hippocampal processes have been significantly limited by methodological challenges in acquiring neuroimaging data, particularly task-related functional neuroimaging data, from infants and toddlers. Recent studies in adults have shown neural activity in the brain regions supporting episodic memory during slow-wave sleep using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and fMRI has been increasingly utilized in infancy and early childhood to address other research questions. We review initial evidence and present preliminary data showing the promise of this approach for examining hippocampal contribution to how infants and toddlers remember individual events, and their association with information about the context in which the event occurred. Overall, our review, integrated with the presentation of some preliminary data provides insight on leveraging sleep to gain new perspectives on early memory functioning.

    • Research article

      Understanding the role and function of maternal touch in children with neurodevelopmental disabilities

      Infant Behavior and Development, Volume 58, 2020, Article 101420

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      During the first years of life, maternal touch can serve different functions including facilitation of child’s gaze orientation to faces which is a key precursor for social attention. Although children with neurodevelopmental disability (ND) may have reduced social skills, the role of maternal touch in contributing to gaze orientation to maternal face has not been explored in previous research. In the present study, we investigate the functions of maternal touch in dyads of mothers and children with ND, compared to dyads of children with typical development (TD). Micro-analytic 2-s coding of maternal touch (i.e., affectionate, playful, facilitating, holding, no touch) and children’s gaze orientation (i.e., mother-oriented, object-oriented, non-oriented) occurred during 5-minute interactions between mothers and their 12-to-24-month-old ND and TD children. Dyads were paired for children chronological age and sex. Mothers of children with ND used more touch – especially facilitating and holding types –, compared to mothers of TD peers. Children with ND exhibited more non-orientated gaze compared to TD counterparts. Playful touch associated with increased mother-oriented gaze in children with ND, whereas affectionate and holding touch associated with greater mother-oriented gaze in TD children. Findings suggest that specific types of touch support children’s attention to social partners.

    • Research article

      Lymphoid Tissue and Pathological Influences of Toxicants

      Comprehensive Toxicology, Volume 11, 2018, pp. 322-342

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Toxicologic pathology plays a crucial role in the identification and interpretation of substance-induced health effects. Histology of lymphoid organs is quite sensitive, although it does not flag every model immunotoxic substance. Subtle interferences of toxic compounds, like transmembrane signaling and cell activation, are often not detectable by conventional histology. The immune system by its very dynamic nature manifests a bandwidth in its morphological picture, hence the ‘normal’ state can be quite variable. Interspecies- and interstrain-dependent differences in the histophysiology of lymphoid organs should be taken into account, as well as the functional and morphological changes in lymphoid organs during life. Increased susceptibility to immunotoxicants may occur during periods of marked histophysiological changes in lymphoid organs. The high sensitivity at perinatal age for a number of immunotoxicants has received specific attention in immunotoxicity testing. In the interpretation of effects, it should be noted that particular components of lymphoid organs may be decreased in number (suppressed–involuted) or increased (stimulated–expanded), but that does not necessarily reflect the overall effects on the immune system.

      The chapter addresses the histology of lymphoid organs, including tertiary lymphoid structures. Toxicant-induced pathologies are presented and associated mechanisms of toxicity are discussed.

      What causes stranger anxiety in babies?

      About fear of strangers Fear of strangers is very common. It happens as your baby develops a healthy attachment to familiar people – like you. Because babies prefer familiar adults, they might react to strangers by crying or fussing, going very quiet, looking fearful or hiding.

      Which of the following situations is most likely to produce stranger anxiety in an infant?

      Terms in this set (30) Which of the following situations is most likely to produce stranger anxiety in an infant? Meeting a stranger in a research laboratory.

      When does a child develop stranger anxiety?

      Stranger anxiety is a perfectly normal developmental stage that often begins around 6 to 8 months. Stranger anxiety typically peaks between 12 and 15 months and then begins to gradually decrease as your baby continues to grow and develop.

      Which is a factor impacting stranger wariness anxiety )?

      Factors related to this variation include the infant's temperament, level of caregiver anxiety, and the developing infant's capacity for the regulation of emotional arousal as measured by physiological markers such as respiratory sinus arrhythmia suppression.